Stay up to date with notifications from The Independent

Notifications can be managed in browser preferences.

Least gifted children of wealthy parents graduate from college at higher rates than most-gifted from low-income families, study finds

Research shows while genetic 'talent' is distributed almost equally, success is not

Andrew van Dam
Tuesday 16 October 2018 10:11 BST
Comments
Researchers noted children from high-income families are likely to perform better from an early age, despite similarities with raw genetic potential
Researchers noted children from high-income families are likely to perform better from an early age, despite similarities with raw genetic potential (Rex)

Your support helps us to tell the story

From reproductive rights to climate change to Big Tech, The Independent is on the ground when the story is developing. Whether it's investigating the financials of Elon Musk's pro-Trump PAC or producing our latest documentary, 'The A Word', which shines a light on the American women fighting for reproductive rights, we know how important it is to parse out the facts from the messaging.

At such a critical moment in US history, we need reporters on the ground. Your donation allows us to keep sending journalists to speak to both sides of the story.

The Independent is trusted by Americans across the entire political spectrum. And unlike many other quality news outlets, we choose not to lock Americans out of our reporting and analysis with paywalls. We believe quality journalism should be available to everyone, paid for by those who can afford it.

Your support makes all the difference.

The least-gifted children of high-income parents graduate from college (university) at higher rates than the most-gifted children of low-income parents, according to new research.

The revolution in genomics which is creeping into economics, therefore allows us to say something we might have suspected, but could never confirm: money trumps genes.

Using one new genome-based measure, economists found genetic endowments are distributed almost equally among children in low-income and high-income families. Success is not.

The least-gifted children of high-income parents graduate from college (university) at higher rates than the most-gifted children of low-income parents.

First, consider the people whose genome scores in the top quarter on a genetic index the researchers associated with educational achievement.

Only about 24 per cent of people born to low-income fathers in that high-potential group graduate from college.

That is dwarfed by the 63 per cent college graduation rate of people with similar genetic scores who are lucky enough to be born to high-income fathers.

Contrast that with a finding from the other end of the genetic scoring scale: about 27 per cent of those who score at the bottom quarter of the genetic index, but are born to high-income fathers, graduate from college. That means they are at least as likely to graduate from college as the highest-scoring low-income students.

The application of genetics to economics is in its infancy. Limitations abound. Most notably, researchers are forced to focus on white people. The world's genomic data comes overwhelmingly from people of European descent and genetic comparisons across races can produce bizarre results.

But it can already begin to expose truths about the economy. The figures above come from a new genome-based study of economic data which aims straight at the heart of the popular conception of America as a meritocracy.

(The Washington Post
(The Washington Post (The Washington Post)

"It goes against the narrative that there are substantial genetic differences between people who are born into wealthy households and those born into poverty," said Kevin Thom, a New York University economist and author of a related working paper released recently by the National Bureau of Economic Research.

"If you don't have the family resources, even the bright kids - the kids who are naturally gifted - are going to have to face uphill battles," Mr Thom said.

"Their potential is being wasted. And that's not good for them, but that's also not good for the economy," his collaborator, Johns Hopkins economist Nicholas Papageorge said. "All those people who didn't go to college who had those high genetic scores, could they have cured cancer?"

Mr Thom and Mr Papageorge's analysis builds on the findings of one of the biggest genome-wide studies yet conducted. Published by a separate team of a dozen authors in Nature Genetics in July, it is the latest result of a lengthy, ongoing effort to bring genetic analysis to the social sciences.

The team scanned millions of individual base pairs across 1,131,881 individual genomes for evidence of correlations between genes and years of schooling completed. They synthesised the findings into a single score we can use to predict educational attainment based on genetic factors.

Mr Thom and Mr Papageorge studied the team's index after it was calculated for a long-running retirement survey sponsored by the Social Security Administration and the National Institute on Ageing. About 20,000 of the survey's respondents, born between 1905 and 1964, provided their DNA along with their responses, which allowed the economists to attach genetic scores individuals' academic and economic achievements.

You already know their key finding, that being born rich trumps being born gifted. But that simple finding becomes more interesting as you come to understand what makes it unique, what it represents and the important ways in which it remains limited.

Studies fuelled by huge genetic data sets likely will not upend economics like they did the biological sciences, but they do allow economists to do something new: control for the environments that people grow up in.

Previous attempts to separate academic potential from the advantages given to children of wealthy families relied on measures such as IQ tests, which are biased by parents' education, occupation and income.

Such tests cannot be administered at conception, birth or infancy - before high-income parents have given their young children a head start by feeding them well, reading to them at higher rates and enrolling them in more activities.

"Two people who are genetically similar can have strikingly different IQ test scores because the richer ones have invested more in their kids," Mr Papageorge said. When you look at the raw genetic potential of the two people, though, "you see they're actually quite similar."

The analysis does not hinge on a "smart gene." Such a thing does not exist. Genes interact in mysterious ways.

Rather than linking individual lines of genetic code to specific characteristics, scientists seek correlations along the 10 million or so steps on the double-helix ladder which explain most human diversity. They focus not on what each base pair might do, but what they might explain in the aggregate.

Geneticists, who had focused on biological attributes with clear genetic connections, were initially sceptical that an outcome as complex as education could be connected to a genetic index, Mr Thom said. But outside tests have consistently proven the score can predict college graduation rates.

Some of the individual genetic encodings influence traits including foetal brain development and lifelong neurotransmitter secretion. Each has an infinitesimal impact on an individual's achievement. Taken together, they explain 11-13 per cent of the difference in academic achievement between people.

That variation is useless if you just want to use your kid's 23andMe data to determine whether she will get the PhD that you never did. But combined with a large population, it allows researchers to do things that, just a few years ago, did not seem possible.

"We are just starting to enter an era where researchers are finding for the first time really credible linkages between genetic markers and social science outcomes," Mr Thom said.

Mr Thom was drawn into the emerging field when colleagues got him interested in genetic markers that offered a biological measure of elevated smoking risk.

"Once I got in there, it's clear that there are all kinds of questions" we could answer with genetic analysis, he said.

The techniques Mr Thom and Mr Papageorge used cannot yet be widely applied. Unlike the retirement survey they used, most economic data does not have a genetic component. Major economic surveys like those behind the unemployment rate are not distributed with little saliva collection kits for genome sequencing.

Their work also has limitations, in addition to being limited to white people for now. The genetic scores they used are hard to separate from their environment. They reflect the genes that were most successful when and where those individuals were growing up. And behaviour that led to academic success decades ago may not be so useful when pedagogy evolves.

When the genetic index Mr Thom and Mr Papageorge used was tested among genomes from siblings, the results indicated as much as a quarter of the variation in score could be due to genetic code that correlates with environmental factors.

There may be genes associated with parenting behaviour that create an environment conducive to your child's success. Children with that gene would tend to succeed in school not because that gene directly helped them study, but because they received both the gene and a success-friendly environment from their parents.

It is a reminder that the study's key finding is also its key caveat: genes are not destiny. Most achievement cannot be explained by genetic factors. Environmental factors like parents' income, on the other hand?

The Washington Post

Join our commenting forum

Join thought-provoking conversations, follow other Independent readers and see their replies

Comments

Thank you for registering

Please refresh the page or navigate to another page on the site to be automatically logged inPlease refresh your browser to be logged in