Stay up to date with notifications from The Independent

Notifications can be managed in browser preferences.

Man blind for 40 years tells of struggle to adapt

Steve Connor
Monday 25 August 2003 00:00 BST
Comments

Your support helps us to tell the story

From reproductive rights to climate change to Big Tech, The Independent is on the ground when the story is developing. Whether it's investigating the financials of Elon Musk's pro-Trump PAC or producing our latest documentary, 'The A Word', which shines a light on the American women fighting for reproductive rights, we know how important it is to parse out the facts from the messaging.

At such a critical moment in US history, we need reporters on the ground. Your donation allows us to keep sending journalists to speak to both sides of the story.

The Independent is trusted by Americans across the entire political spectrum. And unlike many other quality news outlets, we choose not to lock Americans out of our reporting and analysis with paywalls. We believe quality journalism should be available to everyone, paid for by those who can afford it.

Your support makes all the difference.

A man who regained his sight after 40 years of blindness has told scientists about the extraordinary sensation of being able to see once again.

A man who regained his sight after 40 years of blindness has told scientists about the extraordinary sensation of being able to see once again.

Michael May, an American businessman aged 43, went blind when he was three, but sight to his right eye was restored after a pioneering stem-cell transplant in 2000. Tests and investigations using a brain scanner have shown that while Mr May can see colours and shapes, he still has difficulty understanding the visual world around him.

For instance, Mr May was a good downhill skier - following the verbal instructions of a guide - but now he finds it difficult to ski with his eyes open. He also has difficulty recognising faces, and still cannot recognise his wife by her face alone.

"The difference today and two years ago is that I can better guess at what I am seeing. What is the same is that I am still guessing," Mr May said. "A day doesn't go by that I don't appreciate the visual details around me. I have been building my visual catalogue of these details and although this catalogue is significantly more filled out than it was two years ago, there seems to be an infinite number of visuals to absorb," he said.

Cases of restored eyesight are extremely rare and Mr May's recovery is one of the most studied examples of the phenomenon, said Ione Fine of the University of California at San Diego, who led the study published in the journal Nature Neuroscience.

"He's the first case I know of where sight was transformed from bare light perception to very good optical acuity after such a long period of blindness," Dr Fine said.

Even though Mr May can now see, he cannot interpret the world, which he sees in terms of abstract shapes and colours rather than the recognisable objects and familiar faces that most people instantly recognise and take for granted. "What we knew going into this research was that people who regained their sight later in life, after decades of blindness, seemingly experience more difficulties in adapting to and functioning in the visual world than those individuals who lost and regained their sight later in life," Dr Fine said.

The research on Mr May suggests that the ability to cope with motion and to recognise complex shapes such as faces develops later in childhood. The ability may also be "hardwired" in the brain, which makes it difficult to learn when someone is older.

Mr May had to close his eyes while skiing because the sense of moving unsettled him. "I think the sensation of motion overwhelmed him at first, it took him a while to process the sensations of looming so he didn't feel he was going to collide," Dr Fine added.

After two years of practising, however, Mr May can now ski with his eyes open, and has begun to use the patterns of light and shade to estimate the shape of the ski slope. But he cannot yet identify people by their faces alone. He has to use other visual cues, such as their walk, the length of hair or the shape of their eyebrows, to make a guess at who they are.

The scientists said Mr May's experience demonstrated the complex development of sight during early childhood, which involved more than simply sending visual signals from the eyes to the brain.

Join our commenting forum

Join thought-provoking conversations, follow other Independent readers and see their replies

Comments

Thank you for registering

Please refresh the page or navigate to another page on the site to be automatically logged inPlease refresh your browser to be logged in