Waking up to childhood dreams

UNDER THE MICROSCOPE

Susan Greenfield
Sunday 22 December 1996 00:02 GMT
Comments

Your support helps us to tell the story

From reproductive rights to climate change to Big Tech, The Independent is on the ground when the story is developing. Whether it's investigating the financials of Elon Musk's pro-Trump PAC or producing our latest documentary, 'The A Word', which shines a light on the American women fighting for reproductive rights, we know how important it is to parse out the facts from the messaging.

At such a critical moment in US history, we need reporters on the ground. Your donation allows us to keep sending journalists to speak to both sides of the story.

The Independent is trusted by Americans across the entire political spectrum. And unlike many other quality news outlets, we choose not to lock Americans out of our reporting and analysis with paywalls. We believe quality journalism should be available to everyone, paid for by those who can afford it.

Your support makes all the difference.

When He was very small, my brother was convinced that there was a dragon in the bathroom. Although he could follow the repeated rationale from the rest of an exasperated family that dragons did not exist, least of all in Chiswick, the soaring cliff-face of his fear remained unassailed. Although it is easy to scoff at an infantile perspective of reality, might dreams be in some way a temporary turning back of the clock to the bewildering consciousness of childhood?

It might seem odd to think of dreaming as a type of consciousness: but as far as the brain is concerned, dreaming has much more in common with being awake than with dreamless sleep. For example, large protein molecules, so important for everyday brain function, appear to be stock-piled whilst we sleep, or at least while our brains are in dreamless sleep. Experiments show that during the critical stage of sleep which is associated with dreaming, when that the eyes flick rapidly back and forth (rapid eye movement or REM sleep), protein manufacture plummets to that of normal consciousness.

By the same token, brain waves recorded from electrodes on the surface of the scalp during dreamless sleep, take the form of smooth waves of electrical signals undulating, as masses of brain cells all work together in harmonious unison. But during wakefulness and REM sleep, a completely different picture emerges: each brain cell does its own thing. This neuronal assertiveness gives rise to a desynchronized record of "fast" waves on the electroencephalogram (EEG) record, which is virtually indistinguishable between waking and dreaming states.

As far as the brain is concerned, then, dreaming is effectively wakefulness: but as we all know a dream offers a very different type of consciousness to the pleasing and reassuring continuity of a waking experience. In dreams, adult logic no longer applies. And a dream is very rarely of a specific event that can be identified within a particular time and space frame of reference. Instead of elaborate, abstracted thoughts, dreaming amounts to a far more literal consciousness, one reactive to the immediate here and now, where we are no longer fully in control. How similar to a small child's outlook where one is much more the victim of the moment, a passive recipient of bombardment by the senses. In dreaming, and arguably in childhood, one lurches from the pull of one isolated and urgent event to the next. So, perhaps dreaming could be viewed in some way as recapitulating the consciousness of childhood - it is a type of consciousness that could be characterised as more flimsy, less robust than in our adult day-time lives.

A few months ago, I suggested in these pages that fetal consciousness might be best understood using the model of a "dimmer switch", where consciousness grows in degree as the brain grows. The ruptures in continuity, and focus on the here-and-now that can characterise dreams, might then be viewed as hallmarks of a smaller degree of consciousness, a consciousness comparable to that of children, where the brain is served with far fewer connections between neurons. Consistent with these parallels is the observation that the younger the brain, the more time spent in REM steep.

If you go along with the similarities between childhood and dreams, then the dimmer-switch model of consciousness makes perfect sense. Perhaps at night, our diurnal brain chemistry conspires with the under-stimulation of the senses to simulate in the brain temporary conditions similar to those resulting from the sparse connections of childhood: in both cases a smaller degree of consciousness would ensue. Experiencing a nightmare, and indeed the terror that seeps into the first few moments of waking from it, might merely indicate a more modest degree of consciousness, a consciousness where one's personalised neuronal connectivity forged through life's experiences, is sporadically out of commission.

! Susan Greenfield is a neuroscientist at the University of Oxford, and Gresham Professor of Physic, London

Join our commenting forum

Join thought-provoking conversations, follow other Independent readers and see their replies

Comments

Thank you for registering

Please refresh the page or navigate to another page on the site to be automatically logged inPlease refresh your browser to be logged in