Mice playing VR games shed light on how long-term memories are stored in brain
Researchers discovered the brain region linking short-term memory to long-term memory.
Your support helps us to tell the story
From reproductive rights to climate change to Big Tech, The Independent is on the ground when the story is developing. Whether it's investigating the financials of Elon Musk's pro-Trump PAC or producing our latest documentary, 'The A Word', which shines a light on the American women fighting for reproductive rights, we know how important it is to parse out the facts from the messaging.
At such a critical moment in US history, we need reporters on the ground. Your donation allows us to keep sending journalists to speak to both sides of the story.
The Independent is trusted by Americans across the entire political spectrum. And unlike many other quality news outlets, we choose not to lock Americans out of our reporting and analysis with paywalls. We believe quality journalism should be available to everyone, paid for by those who can afford it.
Your support makes all the difference.Scientists have analysed the brains of mice playing virtual reality (VR) games with the aim to learn more about how long-term memories are stored.
The rodents ran on a rotating styrofoam ball – much like a treadmill – while viewing mazes in VR.
The mice received high rewards – unlimited sugar water – for making certain turns in the maze.
When the turns were different, it resulted in either only a few drops of sugar water or a puff of air to the face.
Josue Regalado, a PhD student at Rockefeller University in the US, said: “We structured the virtual reality tasks so that they required a lot of engagement from the mouse in order to start the trial, run through the mazes and get the rewards.
“The more explicit and cognitive the task, the more we’re able to look at how the different brain regions are engaged.”
After the mice learned the three different scenarios, they were assessed again a few weeks later.
The team found the mice were still scampering toward the areas in the maze that had resulted in high rewards.
This was a clear indication that they only remembered decisions that had resulted in big rewards, the team added.
Mr Regalado said: “The key was that the mice could learn all three outcomes in the short-term – very high rewarding, low rewarding, and adverse – but only the high reward would be remembered a month out, because it was the most salient memory.
“That way, we could measure the differences in the neural circuit when recording memories that will be conserved.”
The researchers then monitored the brain activity of the mice while they were performing the tasks.
They focused on two areas of the brain – the hippocampus, where short-term memories are stored, and the anterior thalamus, which regulates emotions.
The aim was to find out how short-term memories in the hippocampus became long-term memories stored in the anterior cingulate cortex, which is involved with emotion formation, learning and memory.
Findings indicated the anterior thalamus played a role in consolidating memories deemed rewarding, which were then were passed on to the cortex.
Andrew C Toader, also a PhD student at the Rockefeller University, said: “The analogy would be your birthday dinner versus the dinner you had three Tuesdays ago.
“You’re more likely to remember what you had on your birthday because it’s more rewarding for you — all your friends are there, it’s exciting — versus just a typical dinner, which you might remember the next day but probably not a month later.”
As part of the next steps, the researchers are planning to investigate how the anterior thalamus contributes to long-term memory storage.
Priya Rajasethupathy, a neuroscientist and assistant professor at the Rockefeller University, said: “The question is what the thalamus does when a memory is valuable and needs to be saved long-term, that it doesn’t do for less valuable memories.”
The research is published in the journal Cell.